Here鈥檚 a Good Indicator You鈥檒l Have Their Attention

News subtitle

Synchronizing to a beat predicts how well a person gets 鈥渋n sync鈥 with others.

Image
Image
Fake pink brain with headphones
(Photo by Anton Watman, Shutterstock) 
Body

How well you synchronize to a simple beat predicts how well you synchronize with another mind, according to a new 天美麻豆 study published this month in .

 has demonstrated that the pupil dilation patterns of speakers and listeners synchronize spontaneously, illustrating shared attention. A team of 天美麻豆 researchers set out to understand how the tendency to synchronize in this way may vary at the individual level and generalize across contexts, as it has been widely debated whether one form of synchrony bears any relationship to another.

鈥淲e were quite surprised to find that how well your pupils dilate and constrict to something as simple as a rhythmic beat would predict how well you attend in the same way as another person,鈥 says lead author Sophie Wohltjen, Guarini 鈥22, who was a graduate student in psychological and brain sciences at 天美麻豆 at the time of the study and is now a postdoctoral researcher at University of Wisconsin-Madison. 鈥淲hat this suggests is that there may be some sort of underlying mechanism that can unite a lot of the different ways that we talk about synchrony.鈥

The research involved two studies. In the first study, individuals listened to a series of tones and were asked to indicate which one was softer than the others while their pupil responses were tracked. Each individual completed this 鈥渙ddball detection task鈥 during nine separate sessions that were held on a different day and time for each session.

The researchers found stable, individually-specific variation in the amount each person entrained to the oddball rhythm. Some people鈥檚 pupils dilated strongly in time with the beat, others less so, and however strongly a person synchronized one day predicted how strongly they synchronized the next.

In the second study, 82 individuals completed the oddball task once and also listened to audio recordings of four emotional stories while their pupil responses were tracked. The storytellers鈥 pupil dilations were recorded earlier when they read the stories. The researchers calculated the pupillary synchrony between the storyteller and listener and then compared this synchrony to how strongly the listener synchronized to the rhythmic beat of the oddball task.

Image
Neuroscience researchers Sophie Wohltjen, Guarini 鈥21, and Professor Thalia Wheatley
Neuroscience researchers Sophie Wohltjen, Guarini 鈥22, and Professor Thalia Wheatley. (Photo by Eli Burakian 鈥00)

The results demonstrate that the more someone entrained to the rhythmic beat of the task, the more likely they were to synchronize their pupils with those of the storyteller. As these individuals could not see the storyteller, pupillary synchrony could not be explained as simple visual mimicry. Instead, this synchrony was evidence that the storyteller and listener were attending to the story in the same way. 

鈥淚dentifying that these two forms of synchrony鈥攕imple, metronomic entrainment and complex shared attention鈥攁re linked is really interesting, as it opens up all sorts of larger questions about why this tendency to synchronize varies between people,鈥 says senior author , the Lincoln Filene Professor in Human Relations and director of the .

鈥淒o musicians synchronize their attention more easily with others? Why are some people super-synchronizers while others are unable to synchronize altogether? Do strong synchronizers find it easier to click with others? These are all questions we plan to investigate further,鈥 says Wheatley.

鈥淭his simple measure of being able to entrain to a beat could have clinical implications for autism and other disorders, which are not only about having difficulty with social interaction but are also about timing,鈥 adds Wheatley.

The research on synchrony to a beat builds on the team鈥檚 , which finds that making and breaking eye contact is linked to fluctuations of pupillary synchrony between conversation partners and makes conversation more engaging.

Amy Olson